-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 1
Commit
This commit does not belong to any branch on this repository, and may belong to a fork outside of the repository.
Organize QMD files to keep main dir from getting too messy. (Renaming updates cache which resaves .RData files, no actual changes to contents)
- Loading branch information
1 parent
591f6c3
commit cdd5e5d
Showing
14 changed files
with
98 additions
and
86 deletions.
There are no files selected for viewing
This file contains bidirectional Unicode text that may be interpreted or compiled differently than what appears below. To review, open the file in an editor that reveals hidden Unicode characters.
Learn more about bidirectional Unicode characters
This file contains bidirectional Unicode text that may be interpreted or compiled differently than what appears below. To review, open the file in an editor that reveals hidden Unicode characters.
Learn more about bidirectional Unicode characters
Original file line number | Diff line number | Diff line change |
---|---|---|
@@ -0,0 +1,3 @@ | ||
# Abstract {.unnumbered} | ||
|
||
Singular *they* is becoming increasingly common and accepted, but many people find it difficult to learn, instead making seemingly-counterintuitive errors like “she uses they/them pronouns.” Existing pronoun production models argue that speakers select pronouns based on morphosyntactic information associated with a name, or based on semantic/conceptual knowledge about a person. However, learning to use singular *they* may require a change in production processes. The first three experiments argue for a model where speakers may instead need to retrieve information from episodic memory about a person’s stated pronouns or which pronouns other speakers use to refer to them. Experiment 1 establishes a measure for how people learn to associate pronouns with a person, and how they use this information to select which pronouns to produce. Experiment 2 investigates how providing people with information about why paying attention to gendered language is important and how seeing singular *they* modeled can support memory for and production of singular *they*. Experiment 3 moves from written to spoken production, testing how including pronouns on nametags and in introductions---common EDI recommendations---affects pronoun choice. Experiment 4 investigates online comprehension, using the visual world paradigm to characterize how singular *they* is being integrated into existing processing mechanisms. The results demonstrate that people can learn that someone uses they/them after only brief introductions, and that a PSA, including pronouns on nametags, and including pronouns in introductions can support accurate production of singular *they*. These findings help extend processing models to account for a wider range of people’s language use and contribute to evidence-based best practices for creating gender-inclusive environments. |
Large diffs are not rendered by default.
Oops, something went wrong.
This file contains bidirectional Unicode text that may be interpreted or compiled differently than what appears below. To review, open the file in an editor that reveals hidden Unicode characters.
Learn more about bidirectional Unicode characters
This file contains bidirectional Unicode text that may be interpreted or compiled differently than what appears below. To review, open the file in an editor that reveals hidden Unicode characters.
Learn more about bidirectional Unicode characters
Oops, something went wrong.